SENSATION AND
PERCEPTION
Vision
Researchers have studied vision more
thoroughly than the other senses. Because people need sight to perform most
daily activities, the sense of sight has evolved to be highly sophisticated.
Vision, however, would not exist without the presence of light. Light is
electromagnetic radiation that travels in the form of waves. Light is emitted
from the sun, stars, fire, and lightbulbs. Most other objects just reflect
light.
People experience light as having three
features: color,brightness, and saturation.
These three types of experiences come from three corresponding characteristics
of light waves:
·
The color or hue of light depends on its wavelength,
the distance between the peaks of its waves.
·
The brightness of light is related to intensity or the amount of
light an object emits or reflects. Brightness depends on light wave
amplitude, the height of light waves. Brightness is also somewhat
influenced by wavelength. Yellow light tends to look brighter than reds or
blues.
·
Saturation or colorfulness depends on light complexity,
the range of wavelengths in light. The color of a single wavelength is pure
spectral color. Such lights are called fully saturated. Outside a laboratory,
light is rarely pure or of a single wavelength. Light is usually a mixture of
several different wavelengths. The greater number of spectral colors in a
light, the lower the saturation. Light of mixed wavelengths looks duller or
paler than pure light.
Wavelength ——> Color
|
Amplitude ——> Brightness
|
Complexity ——> Saturation
|
Rainbows and Lights
White light: Completely unsaturated. It is a mixture of
all wavelengths of light.
The visible spectrum: Includes the colors
of the rainbow, which are red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet.
Ultraviolet light: The kind of light
that causes sunburns. It has a wavelength somewhat shorter than the violet
light at the end of the visible spectrum.
Infrared radiation: Has a wavelength
somewhat longer than the red light at the other end of the visible spectrum.
Structure of the Eye
The process of vision cannot be understood
without some knowledge about the structure of the eye:
·
The cornea is the transparent, protective outer
membrane of the eye.
·
The iris, the colored part of the eye, is a ring of
muscle.
·
The iris surrounds an opening called the pupil,
which can get bigger or smaller to allow different amounts of light through the
lens to the back of the eye. In bright light, the pupil contracts to restrict
light intake; in dim light, the pupil expands to increase light intake.
·
The lens, which lies behind the pupil and iris, can
adjust its shape to focus light from objects that are near or far away. This
process is called accommodation.
·
Light passing through the cornea, pupil, and lens falls onto the
retina at the back of the eye. The retina is a thin layer of
neural tissue. The image that falls on the retina is always upside down.
·
The center of the retina, the fovea, is where vision
is sharpest. This explains why people look directly at an object they want to
inspect. This causes the image to fall onto the fovea, where vision is
clearest.
Eye Trouble
Nearsightedness is the inability to clearly
see distant objects. Farsightedness is the inability to clearly see close
objects. A cataract is a lens that has become opaque, resulting in impaired
vision.
Rods and Cones
The retina has millions of photoreceptors
called rods and cones. Photoreceptorsare specialized cells that
respond to light stimuli. There are many more rods than cones. The long, narrow
cells, called rods, are highly sensitive to light and allow vision
even in dim conditions. There are no rods in the fovea, which is why vision
becomes hazy in dim light. However, the area just outside the fovea contains
many rods, and these allow peripheral vision.
Because rods are so sensitive to light, in dim
lighting conditions peripheral vision is sharper than direct vision.
Example: People can often see a
star in the night sky if they look a little to the side of the star instead of
directly at it. Looking to the side utilizes peripheral vision and makes the
image of the star fall onto the periphery of the retina, which contains most of
the rods.
Cones are cone-shaped cells that can
distinguish between different wavelengths of light, allowing people to see in
color. Cones don’t work well in dim light, however, which is why people have
trouble distinguishing colors at night. The fovea has only cones, but as the
distance from the fovea increases, the number of cones decreases.
Feature
|
Rods
|
Cones
|
Shape
|
Long and narrow
|
Cone-shaped
|
Sensitivity to light
|
High: help people to see in dim
light
|
Low: help people to see in bright
light
|
Help color vision
|
No
|
Yes
|
Present in fovea
|
No
|
Yes
|
Abundant in periphery of retina
|
Yes
|
No
|
Allow peripheral vision
|
Yes
|
No
|
Adaptation to Light
Dark adaptation is the process
by which receptor cells sensitize to light, allowing clearer vision in dim
light. Light adaptation is the process by which receptor cells
desensitize to light, allowing clearer vision in bright light.
Connection to the
Optic Nerve
Rods and cones connect via synapses to bipolar
neurons, which then connect to other neurons called ganglion cells. The axons
of all the ganglion cells in the retina come together to make up the optic
nerve. The optic nerve connects to the eye at a spot in the retina called
the optic disk. The optic disk is also called the blind spot
because it has no rods or cones. Any image that falls on the blind spot
disappears from view.
Transmission of Visual
Information
Visual information travels from the eye to the
brain as follows:
·
Light reflected from an object hits the retina’s rods and cones.
·
Rods and cones send neural signals to the bipolar cells.
·
Bipolar cells send signals to the ganglion cells.
·
Ganglion cells send signals through the optic nerve to the brain.
Bipolar and ganglion cells gather and compress
information from a large number of rods and cones. The rods and cones that send
information to a particular bipolar or ganglion cell make up that cell’s
receptive field.
Ganglion cell axons from the inner half of
each eye cross over to the opposite half of the brain. This means that each
half of the brain receives signals from both eyes. Signals from the eyes’ left
sides go to the left side of the brain, and signals from the eyes’ right sides
go to the right side of the brain. The diagram below illustrates this process.
Visual Processing in
the Brain
After being processed in the thalamus and
different areas of the brain, visual signals eventually reach the primary
visual cortex in the occipital lobe of the brain’s cerebrum. In the 1960s,
David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel demonstrated that highly specialized cells
called feature detectors respond to these visual signals in
the primary visual cortex. Feature detectors are neurons that respond to
specific features of the environment, such as lines and edges.
From the visual cortex, visual signals often
travel on to other parts of the brain, where more processing occurs. Cells
deeper down the visual processing pathway are even more specialized than those
in the visual cortex. Psychologists theorize that perception occurs when a
large number of neurons in different parts of the brain activate. These neurons
may respond to various features of the perceived object such as edges, angles,
shapes, movement, brightness, and texture.
Color Vision
Objects in the world seem to be brightly
colored, but they actually have no color at all. Red cars, green leaves, and
blue sweaters certainly exist—but their color is a psychological experience.
Objects only produce or reflect light of different wavelengths and amplitudes.
Our eyes and brains then convert this light information to experiences of
color. Color vision happens because of two different processes, which occur in
sequence:
·
The first process occurs in the retina and is explained by the
trichromatic theory.
·
The second process occurs in retinal ganglion cells and in cells
in the thalamus and visual cortex. The opponent process theory explains this
process.
These two theories are explained below.
The Trichromatic
Theory
Thomas Young and Hermann von Helmholtz proposed
the trichromatic theory, or Young-Helmholtz theory.
This theory states that the retina contains three types of cones, which respond
to light of three different wavelengths, corresponding to red, green, or blue.
Activation of these cones in different combinations and to different degrees
results in the perception of other colors.
Color Mixing
Mixing lights of different colors is called
additive color mixing. This process adds wavelengths together and results in
more light. Mixing paints, on the other hand, is called subtractive color
mixing, a process that removes wavelengths so that there is less light. If red,
orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet light were mixed, the result
would be white light. If the same color paints were mixed together, the result
would be a dark, muddy color.
The trichromatic theory also accounts
for color blindness, a hereditary condition that affects a person’s
ability to distinguish between colors. Most color-blind people are dichromats,
which means they are sensitive to only two of the three wavelengths of light.
Dichromats are usually insensitive either to red or green, but sometimes they
cannot see blue.
The Opponent Process
Theory
Ewald Hering proposed the opponent process theory.
According to this theory, the visual system has receptors that react in
opposite ways to three pairs of colors. The three pairs of colors are red
versus green, blue versus yellow, and black versus white. Some receptors are
activated by wavelengths corresponding to red light and are turned off by
wavelengths corresponding to green light. Other receptors are activated by
yellow light and turned off by blue light. Still others respond oppositely to
black and white.
Opponent process theory explains why most
people perceive four primary colors: red, green, blue, and yellow. If
trichromatic theory alone fully explained color vision, people would perceive
only three primary colors, and all other colors would be combinations of these
three colors. However, most people think of yellow as primary rather than as a
mixture of colors.
Opponent process theory also accounts for
complementary or negative afterimages. Afterimages are colors
perceived after other, complementary colors are removed.
Example: If Jack stares at a
picture of a red square, wavelengths corresponding to red will activate the
matching receptors in his visual system. For the sake of simplicity, these
matching receptors can be referred to as red receptors. Anything that makes red
receptors increase firing will be seen as red, so Jack will see the square as
red. Anything that decreases the firing of red receptors will be seen as green.
If Jack stares at the square for a while, the red receptors will get tired out
and start to fire less. Then if he looks at a blank white sheet of paper, he
will see a green square. The decreased firing of the red receptors produces an
experience of a green afterimage.
Form Perception
The ability to see separate objects or forms
is essential to daily functioning. Suppose a girl sees a couple in the distance
with their arms around each other. If she perceived them as a four-legged,
two-armed, two-headed person, she’d probably be quite disturbed. People can
make sense of the world because the visual system makes sensible
interpretations of the information the eyes pick up.
Gestalt psychology, a school of thought
that arose in Germany in the early twentieth century, explored how people
organize visual information into patterns and forms. Gestalt psychologists
noted that the perceived whole is sometimes more than the sum of its parts. An
example of this is the phi phenomenon, or stroboscopic movement,
which is an illusion of movement that happens when a series of images is
presented very quickly, one after another.
Example: The phi phenomenon is
what gives figures and objects in movies the illusion of movement. In reality,
a movie is a series of still images presented in rapid succession.
Gestalt Principles
Gestalt psychologists described several
principles people use to make sense of what they see. These principles include
figure and ground, proximity, closure, similarity, continuity, and simplicity:
·
Figure and ground: One of the main ways people organize visual
information is to divide what they see into figure and ground. Figure is
what stands out, andground is the background in which the figure
stands. People may see an object as figure if it appears larger or brighter
relative to the background. They may also see an object as figure if it differs
noticeably from the background or if it moves against a static environment.
·
Proximity: When objects lie close together, people tend
to perceive the objects as a group. For example, in the graphic below, people
would probably see these six figures as two groups of three.
·
Closure: People tend to interpret familiar, incomplete
forms as complete by filling in gaps. People can easily recognize the following
figure as the letter k in spite of the gaps.
·
Similarity: People tend to group similar objects together.
In the next figure, people could probably distinguish the letter T because
similar dots are seen as a group.
·
Continuity: When people see interrupted lines and
patterns, they tend to perceive them as being continuous by filling in gaps.
The next figure is seen as a circle superimposed on a continuous line rather
than two lines connected to a circle.
·
Simplicity: People tend to perceive forms as simple,
symmetrical figures rather than as irregular ones. This figure is generally
seen as one triangle superimposed on another rather than a triangle with an
angular piece attached to it.
Depth Perception
To figure out the location of an object,
people must be able to estimate their distance from that object. Two types of
cues help them to do this: binocular cues and monocular cues.
Binocular Cues
Binocular cues are cues that
require both eyes. These types of cues help people to estimate the distance of
nearby objects. There are two kinds of binocular cues: retinal disparity and
convergence.
·
Retinal disparity marks the difference between two images.
Because the eyes lie a couple of inches apart, their retinas pick up slightly
different images of objects. Retinal disparity increases as the eyes get closer
to an object. The brain uses retinal disparity to estimate the distance between
the viewer and the object being viewed.
·
Convergence is when the eyes turn inward to look at an object close
up. The closer the object, the more the eye muscles tense to turn the eyes
inward. Information sent from the eye muscles to the brain helps to determine
the distance to the object.
Monocular Cues
Monocular cues are cues that
require only one eye. Several different types of monocular cues help us to
estimate the distance of objects: interposition, motion parallax, relative size
and clarity, texture gradient, linear perspective, and light and shadow.
·
Interposition: When one object is blocking part of another
object, the viewer sees the blocked object as being farther away.
·
Motion parallax or relative motion: When
the viewer is moving, stationary objects appear to move in different directions
and at different speeds depending on their location. Relatively close objects
appear to move backward. The closer the object, the faster it appears to move.
Distant objects appear to move forward. The further away the object, the slower
it appears to move.
·
Relative size: People see objects that make a smaller image
on the retina as farther away.
·
Relative clarity: Objects that appear sharp, clear, and detailed
are seen as closer than more hazy objects.
·
Texture gradient: Smaller objects that are more thickly
clustered appear farther away than objects that are spread out in space.
·
Linear perspective: Parallel lines that converge appear far away.
The more the lines converge, the greater the perceived distance.
·
Light and shadow: Patterns of light and shadow make objects
appear three-dimensional, even though images of objects on the retina are
two-dimensional.
Creating Perspective
Artists use monocular cues to give a
three-dimensional appearance to two-dimensional pictures. For instance, if an
artist wanted to paint a landscape scene with a straight highway on it, she
would show the edges of the highway as two parallel lines gradually coming
together to indicate that the highway continues into the distance. If she
wanted to paint cars on the highway, she would paint bigger cars if she wanted
them to seem closer and smaller cars if she wanted them to seem farther away.
Perceptual Constancy
Another important ability that helps people
make sense of the world is perceptual constancy. Perceptual constancy is
the ability to recognize that an object remains the same even when it produces
different images on the retina.
Example: When a man watches his
wife walk away from him, her image on his retina gets smaller and smaller, but
he doesn’t assume she’s shrinking. When a woman holds a book in front of her
face, its image is a rectangle. However, when she puts it down on the table,
its image is a trapezoid. Yet she knows it’s the same book.
Although perceptual constancy relates to other
senses as well, visual constancy is the most studied phenomenon. Different
kinds of visual constancies relate to shape, color, size, brightness, and
location.
·
Shape constancy: Objects appear to have the same shape even
though they make differently shaped retinal images, depending on the viewing
angle.
·
Size constancy: Objects appear to be the same size even though
their images get larger or smaller as their distance decreases or increases.
Size constancy depends to some extent on familiarity with the object. For
example, it is common knowledge that people don’t shrink. Size constancy also
depends on perceived distance. Perceived size and perceived distance are
strongly related, and each influences the other.
·
Brightness constancy: People see objects as having the same
brightness even when they reflect different amounts of light as lighting
conditions change.
·
Color constancy: Different wavelengths of light are reflected
from objects under different lighting conditions. Outdoors, objects reflect
more light in the blue range of wavelengths, and indoors, objects reflect more
light in the yellow range of wavelengths. Despite this, people see objects as
having the same color whether they are outdoors or indoors because of two
factors. One factor is that the eyes adapt quickly to different lighting
conditions. The other is that the brain interprets the color of an object
relative to the colors of nearby objects. In effect, the brain cancels out the
extra blueness outdoors and the extra yellowness indoors.
·
Location constancy: Stationary objects don’t appear to move even
though their images on the retina shift as the viewer moves around.
Visual Illusions
The brain uses Gestalt principles, depth
perception cues, and perceptual constancies to make hypotheses about the world.
However, the brain sometimes misinterprets information from the senses and
makes incorrect hypotheses. The result is an optical illusion. An illusion is
a misinterpretation of a sensory stimulus. Illusions can occur in other senses,
but most research has been done on visual illusions.
In the famous Muller-Lyer illusion shown
here, the vertical line on the right looks longer than the line on the left,
even though the two lines are actually the same length.
This illusion is probably due to misinterpretation
of depth perception cues. Because of the attached diagonal lines, the vertical
line on the left looks like the near edge of a building, and the vertical line
on the right looks like the far edge of a room. The brain uses distance cues to
estimate size. The retinal images of both lines are the same size, but since
one appears nearer, the brain assumes that it must be smaller.
Perceptual Set
The Muller-Lyer illusion doesn’t fool everyone
equally. Researchers have found that people who live in cities experience a
stronger illusion than people who live in forests. In other words,
city-dwelling people see the lines as more different in size. This could be
because buildings and rooms surround city dwellers, which prepares them to see
the lines as inside and outside edges of buildings. The difference in the
strength of the illusion could also be due to variations in the amount of
experience people have with making three-dimensional interpretations of
two-dimensional drawings.
Cultural differences in the tendency to see
illusions illustrate the importance of perceptual set. Perceptual set is
the readiness to see objects in a particular way based on expectations,
experiences, emotions, and assumptions. Perceptual set influences our everyday
perceptions and how we perceive reversible figures, which are
ambiguous drawings that can be interpreted in more than one way. For example,
people might see a vase or two faces in this famous figure, depending on what
they’re led to expect.
Selective Attention
Reversible figures also illustrate the concept
of selective attention, the ability to focus on some bits of
sensory information and ignore others. When people focus on the white part of
the figure, they see a vase, and when they focus on the black part of it, they
see two faces. To use the language of Gestalt psychology, people can choose to
make the vase figure and the face ground or vice versa.
Selective attention allows people to carry on
day-to-day activities without being overwhelmed by sensory information. Reading
a book would be impossible if the reader paid attention to not only the words
on the page but also all the things in his peripheral vision, all the sounds
around him, all the smells in the air, all the information his brain gets about
his body position, air pressure, temperature, and so on. He wouldn’t get very
far with the book.
Context Effects
Another factor that influences perception is
the context of the perceiver. People’s immediate surroundings create
expectations that make them see in particular ways.
Example: The figure below can
be seen either as a sequence of letters, A B C, or a sequence of numbers,
12 13 14, depending on whether it is scanned across or down.
Social Plugin