Forgetting
Memory researchers certainly haven’t
forgotten Hermann Ebbinghaus, the first person to do scientific
studies of forgetting, using himself as a subject. He spent a lot of time
memorizing endless lists of nonsense syllables and then testing himself to see
whether he remembered them. He found that he forgot most of what he learned
during the first few hours after learning it.
Later researchers have found that forgetting
doesn’t always occur that quickly. Meaningful information fades more slowly
than nonsense syllables. The rate at which people forget or retain information
also depends on what method is used to measure forgetting and retention. Retention is
the proportion of learned information that is retained or remembered—the flip
side of forgetting.
Forgetting Curve
A forgetting curve is a graph that shows how
quickly learned information is forgotten over time. Ebbinghaus made use of
forgetting curves to chart his research on memory.
Measures of Forgetting
and Retention
Researchers measure forgetting and retention
in three different ways: recall, recognition, and relearning.
Recall
Recall is remembering without any external
cues. For example, essay questions test recall of knowledge because nothing on
a blank sheet of paper will jog the memory.
Recognition
Recognition is identifying learned information using
external cues. For example, true or false questions and multiple-choice
questions test recognition because the previously learned information is there
on the page, along with other options. In general, recognition is easier than
recall.
Relearning
When using the relearning method
to measure retention, a researcher might ask a subject to memorize a long
grocery list. She might measure how long he has to practice before he remembers
every item. Suppose it takes him ten minutes. On another day, she gives him the
same list again and measures how much time he takes to relearn the list.
Suppose he now learns it in five minutes. He has saved five minutes of learning
time, or 50 percent of the original time it took him to learn it. His savings
score of 50 percent indicates that he retained 50 percent of the information he
learned the first time.
Causes of Forgetting
Everyone forgets things. There are six main
reasons for forgetting: ineffective encoding, decay, interference, retrieval
failure, motivated forgetting, and physical injury or trauma.
Ineffective Encoding
The way information is encoded affects
the ability to remember it. Processing information at a deeper level makes it
harder to forget. If a student thinks about the meaning of the concepts in her
textbook rather than just reading them, she’ll remember them better when the
final exam comes around. If the information is not encoded properly—such as if
the student simply skims over the textbook while paying more attention to the
TV—it is more likely to be forgotten.
Decay
According to decay theory, memory
fades with time. Decay explains the loss of memories from sensory and
short-term memory. However, loss of long-term memories does not seem to depend
on how much time has gone by since the information was learned. People might
easily remember their first day in junior high school but completely forget
what they learned in class last Tuesday.
Interference
Interference theory has a better
account of why people lose long-term memories. According to this theory, people
forget information because of interference from other learned information.
There are two types of interference: retroactive and proactive.
·
Retroactive interference happens when newly learned information
makes people forget old information.
·
Proactive interference happens when old information makes
people forget newly learned information.
Retrieval Failure
Forgetting may also result from failure
to retrieve information in memory, such as if the wrong sort
of retrieval cue is used. For example, Dan may not be able to
remember the name of his fifth-grade teacher. However, the teacher’s name might
suddenly pop into Dan’s head if he visits his old grade school and sees his
fifth-grade classroom. The classroom would then be acting as a context cue for
retrieving the memory of his teacher’s name.
Motivated Forgetting
Psychologist Sigmund Freud proposed
that people forget because they push unpleasant or intolerable thoughts and
feelings deep into their unconscious. He called this phenomenon repression.
The idea that people forget things they don’t want to remember is also
called motivated forgetting or psychogenic amnesia.
Physical Injury or
Trauma
Anterograde amnesia is the inability
to remember events that occur after an injury or traumatic event. Retrograde
amnesia is the inability to remember events that occurred before an
injury or traumatic event.
Social Plugin