Student motivation
Not so long ago, a teacher named Barbara Fuller taught general science
to elementary years
students, and one of her units was about insects and spiders. As part
of the unit she had students
search for insects and spiders around their own homes or apartments.
They brought the creatures to school (safely in jars), answered a number of
questions about them in their journals, and eventually gave brief oral reports
about their findings to the class. The assignment seemed straightforward, but Barbara
found that students responded to it in very different ways. Looking back, here
is how Barbara described their responses: “I remember Jose couldn’t wait to get
started, and couldn’t bear to end the assignment either! Every day he brought
more bugs or spiders—eventually 25 different kinds. Every day he drew pictures
of them in his journal and wrote copious notes about them. At the end he gave
the best oral presentation I’ve ever seen from a third-grader; he called it
‘They Have Us Outnumbered!’ I wish I had filmed it, he was so poised and so
enthusiastic. “Then there was Lindsey—the one who was always wanted to be the
best in everything, regardless of whether it interested her. She started off
the work rather slowly—just brought in a few bugs and only one spider. But she
kept an eye on what everyone else was bringing, and how much. When she saw how
much Jose was doing, though, she picked up her pace, like she was trying to
match his level. Except that instead of bringing a diversity of creatures as
Jose was doing, she just brought more and more of the same ones—almost twenty
dead house flies, as I recall! Her presentation was OK—I really could not give
her a bad mark for it—but it wasn’t as creative or insightful as Jose’s. I
think she was more concerned about her mark than about the material. “And there
was Tobias—discouraging old Tobias. He did the work, but just barely. I noticed
him looking a lot at other students’ insect collections and at their journal
entries. He wasn’t cheating, I believe, just figuring out what the basic level
of work was for the assignment—what he needed to do simply to avoid failing it.
He brought in fewer bugs than most others, though still a number that was acceptable.
He also wrote shorter answers in his journal and gave one of the shortest oral
reports. It was all acceptable, but not much more than that. “And Zoey: she was
quite a case! I never knew whether to laugh or cry about her. She didn’t
exactly resist doing the assignment, but she certainly liked to chat with other
students. So she was easily distracted, and that cut down on getting her work
done, especially about her journal entries. What really saved her—what kept her
work at a reasonably high level of quality—were the two girls she ended up
chatting with. The other two were already pretty motivated to do a lot with the
assignment
—create fine looking bug collections, write good journal entries, and
make interesting oral
presentations. So when Zoey attempted chitchat with them, the
conversations often ended up
6. Student
motivation
focusing on the assignment anyway! She had them to thank for keeping
her mind on the work. I don’t know what Zoey would have done without them.”
As Barbara Fuller’s
recollections suggest, students assign various meanings and attitudes to
academic activities
—personal meanings
and attitudes that arouse and direct their energies in different ways. We call
these and their associated energizing and directing effects by the term motivation,
or sometimes motivation to learn. As you will see, differences in
motivation are an important source of diversity in classrooms, comparable in
importance to differences in prior knowledge, ability, or developmental
readiness. When it comes to school learning, furthermore, students’ motivations
take on special importance because students’ mere presence in class is (of
course) no guarantee that students really want to learn. It is only a sign that
students live in a society requiring young people to attend school. Since modern
education is compulsory, teachers cannot take students’ motivation for granted,
and they have a responsibility to insure students’ motivation to learn. Somehow
or other, teachers must persuade students to want to do what students have to
do anyway. This task—understanding and therefore influencing students’
motivations to learn—is the focus of this chapter. Fortunately, as you will
see, there are ways of accomplishing this task that respect students’ choices,
desires, and attitudes.
Like motivation itself,
theories of it are full of diversity. For convenience in navigating through the
diversity, we have organized the chapter around six major theories or
perspectives about motives and their sources. We call the topics (1) motives as
behavior change, (2) motives as goals, (3) motives as interests, (4) motives as
attributions about success, (5) motives as beliefs about self-efficacy, and (6)
motives as self-determination. We end with a perspective called expectancy-value
theory which integrates ideas from some of the other six theories, and
partly as a result implies some additional suggestions for influencing
students’ motivations to learn in positive ways.
Motives
as behavior
Sometimes it is
useful to think of motivation not as something “inside” a student driving the
student’s behavior, but as equivalent to the student’s outward
behaviors. This is the perspective of behaviorism, which we discussed in
Chapter 1 (“Student learning”) as a way to think about the learning process. In
its most thorough-going form, behaviorism focuses almost completely on what can
be directly seen or heard about a person’s behavior, and has relatively few
comments about what may lie behind (or “underneath” or “inside”) the behavior.
When it comes to motivation, this perspective means minimizing or even ignoring
the distinction between the inner drive or energy of students, and the outward
behaviors that express the drive or energy. The two are considered the same, or
nearly so. Equating the inner and the outward might seem to violate common
sense. How can a student do something without some sort of feeling or thought
to make the action happen? As we will explain, this very question has led to
alternative models of motivation that are based on cognitive rather than
behaviorist theories of learning. We will explain some of these later in this
chapter. Before getting to them, however, we encourage you to consider the
advantages of a behaviorist perspective on motivation. Sometimes the
circumstances of teaching limit teachers’ opportunities to distinguish between
inner motivation and outward behavior. Certainly teachers see plenty of student
behaviors—signs of motivation of some sort. But the multiple demands of
teaching can limit the time needed to determine what the behaviors mean. If a
student asks a lot of questions during discussions, for example, is he or she
curious about the material itself, or just wanting to look intelligent in front
of classmates and the teacher? In a class with many students and a busy agenda,
there may not be a lot of time for a teacher to decide between these
possibilities. In other cases, the problem may not be limited time as much as
communication difficulties with a student. Consider a student who is still
learning English, or who belongs to a cultural community that uses patterns of
conversation that are unfamiliar to the teacher, or who has a disability that
limits the student’s general language skill. In these cases discerning the
student’s inner motivations may take more time and effort. It is important to
invest the extra time and effort for such students, but while a teacher is
doing so, it is also important for her to guide and influence the students’
behavior in constructive directions. That is where behaviorist approaches to
motivation can help.
Operant
conditioning as a way of motivating
The most common
version of the behavioral perspective on motivation is the theory of operant
conditioning associated with B. F. Skinner (1938, 1957), which we discussed
in Chapter 1 (“Learning process”). The description in that chapter
focused on behavioral learning, but the same operant model can be transformed
into an account of motivation. In the operant model, you may recall, a
behavior being learned (the “operant”) increases in frequency or
likelihood because performing it makes a reinforcement available. To understand
this model in terms of motivation, think of the likelihood of
response as the motivation and the reinforcement as the motivator.
Imagine, for example, that a student learns by operant conditioning to
answer questions during class discussions: each time the student answers
a question (the operant), the teacher praises (reinforces) this behavior. In
addition to thinking of this situation as behavioral learning,
however, you can also think of it in terms of motivation: the likelihood
of the student answering questions (the motivation) is increasing
because of the teacher’s praise (the motivator).
Many concepts from
operant conditioning, in fact, can be understood in motivational terms. Another
one, for example, is the concept of extinction, which we defined in
Chapter 1 as the tendency for learned behaviors to become less likely when
reinforcement no longer occurs—a sort of “unlearning”, or at least a decrease
in performance of previously learned. The decrease in performance frequency can
be thought of as a loss of motivation, and removal of the reinforcement can be
thought of as removal of the motivator. Table 14 summarizes
this way of reframing
operant conditioning in terms of motivation, both for the concepts discussed in
Chapter 1 and for other additional concepts.
Table 14: Operant
conditioning as learning and as motivation
Concept
|
Definition
phrased
in terms of
learning
|
Definition
phrased
in terms of
motivation
|
Classroom
example
|
Operant
|
Behavior that
becomes
more likely
because of
reinforcement
|
Behavior that
suggests
an increase in
motivation
|
Student
listens to
teacher’s
comments during
lecture or
discussion
|
Reinforcement
|
Stimulus that
increases
likelihood of
a behavior
|
Stimulus that
motivates
|
Teacher
praises student
for listening
|
Positive
reinforcement
|
Stimulus that increases
likelihood of
a behavior by being introduced or added to a situation
|
Stimulus that motivates
by its presence;
an
“incentive”
|
Teacher makes
encouraging
remarks
about
student’s homework
|
Negative
reinforcement
|
Stimulus that increases
the likelihood
of a behavior by being removed or taken
away from a
situation
|
Stimulus that
motivates
by its absence
or
avoidance
|
Teacher stops
nagging
student about
late
homework
|
Punishment
|
Stimulus that decreases
the likelihood of
a behavior by
being
introduced or added to
a
situation
|
Stimulus that
decreases motivation by
its presence
|
Teacher
deducts points
for late
homework
|
Extinction
|
Removal of
reinforcement
for a
behavior
|
Removal of
motivating
stimulus that
leads to
decrease in
motivation
|
Teacher stops
commenting
altogether
about
student’s homework
|
Shaping
successive
approximations
|
Reinforcements
for
behaviors that
gradually
resemble
(approximate) a final goal behavior
|
Stimuli that
gradually
shift
motivation toward a final goal motivation
|
Teacher
praises student
for returning
homework bit closer to thedeadline;
gradually she praises for actually being on time
|
Continuous
reinforcement
|
Reinforcement
that
occurs each
time that an operant behavior occurs
|
Motivator that
occurs
each time that a
behavioral sign of motivation occurs
|
Teacher
praises highly
active student
for every
time he works
for five
minutes
without
interruption
|
Intermittent
reinforcement
|
Reinforcement
that
sometimes occurs
following an
operant
behavior, but
not on every occasion
|
Motivator that
occurs
sometimes when a
behavioral
sign of
motivation
occurs, but not on every occasion
|
Teacher
praises highly
Active student
sometimes when he works without
interruption,
but not every time
|
Cautions about
behavioral perspectives on motivation
As we mentioned,
behaviorist perspectives about motivation do reflect a classroom reality: that teachers
sometimes lack time and therefore must focus simply on students’ appropriate
outward behavior. But there are nonetheless cautions about adopting this view.
An obvious one is the ambiguity of students’ specific behaviors; what looks
like a sign of one motive to the teacher may in fact be a sign of some other
motive to the student (DeGrandpre, 2000). If a student looks at the teacher
intently while she is speaking, does it mean the student is motivated to learn,
or only that the student is daydreaming? If a student invariably looks away
while the teacher is
speaking, does it
mean that the student is disrespectful of the teacher, or that student comes
from a family or cultural group where avoiding eye contact actually
shows more respect for a speaker than direct eye contact? .Another concern
about behaviorist perspectives, including operant conditioning, is that it
leads teachers to ignore students’ choices and preferences, and to “play God”
by making choices on their behalf (Kohn, 1996).
According to this
criticism, the distinction between “inner” motives and expressions of motives
in outward behavior does not disappear just because a teacher (or a
psychological theory) chooses to treat a motive and the behavioral expression
of a motive as equivalent. Students usually do know what they want or
desire, and their wants or desires may not always correspond to what a teacher
chooses to reinforce or ignore. This, in a new guise, is once again the issue
of intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation behavioral, it is
argued, are not sensitive enough to students’ intrinsic, self-sustaining
motivations. As we pointed out in Chapter 1, there is truth to this allegation
if a teacher actually does rely on rewarding behaviors that she alone has
chosen, or even if she persists in reinforcing behaviors that students already
find motivating without external reinforcement. In those cases reinforcements
can backfire: instead of serving as an
incentive to desired
behavior, reinforcement can become a reminder of the teacher’s power and of
students’ lack of control over their own actions. A classic research study of
intrinsic motivation illustrated the problem nicely. In the study, researchers
rewarded university students for two activities—solving puzzles and writing
newspaper headlines —that they already found interesting. Some of the students,
however, were paid to do these activities, whereas others were not.
Under these conditions, the students who were paid were less likely to
engage in the activities following the experiment than were the students who
were not paid, even though both groups had been equally interested in the
activities to begin with (Deci, 1971). The extrinsic reward of payment, it
seemed, interfered with the intrinsic reward of working the puzzles.
Later studies
confirmed this effect in numerous situations, though they have also found
certain conditions where extrinsic rewards do not reduce intrinsic
rewards. Extrinsic rewards are not as harmful, for example, if a person is paid
“by the hour” (i.e. by a flat rate) rather than piecemeal (by the number of
items completed) (Cameron & Pierce, 1994; Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996).
They also are less harmful if the task itself is relatively well-defined (like working
math problems or playing solitaire) and high-quality performance is expected at
all times. So there are still times and ways when externally determined
reinforcements are useful and effective. In general, however, extrinsic rewards
do seem to undermine intrinsic motivation often enough that they need to be used
selectively and thoughtfully (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 2001). As it happens,
help with being selective and thoughtful can be found in the other, more
cognitively oriented theories of motivation. These use the goals, interests,
and beliefs of students as ways of explaining differences in students’ motives
and in how the motives affect engagement with school. We turn to these
cognitively oriented theories next, beginning with those focused on students’
goals.
Motives
as goals
One way motives vary
is by the kind of goals that students set for themselves, and by how the goals
support students’ academic achievement. As you might suspect, some goals
encourage academic achievement more than others, but even motives that do not
concern academics explicitly tend to affect learning indirectly.
6. Student
motivation
Goals that
contribute to achievement
What kinds of
achievement goals do students hold? Imagine three individuals, Maria, Sara, and
Lindsay, who are taking algebra together. Maria’s main concern is to learn the
material as well as possible because she finds it interesting and because she
believes it will be useful to her in later courses, perhaps at university. Hers
is a mastery goal because she wants primarily to learn or master the material.
Sara, however, is concerned less about algebra than about getting top marks on
the exams and in the course. Hers is a performance goal because she is focused primarily
on looking successful; learning algebra is merely a vehicle for performing well
in the eyes of peers and teachers. Lindsay, for her part, is primarily
concerned about avoiding a poor or failing mark. Hers is a performanceavoidance
goal or failure-avoidance goal because she is not really as concerned about
learning algebra, as Maria is, or about competitive success, as Sara is; she is
simply intending to avoid failure. As you might imagine, mastery, performance,
and performance-avoidance goals often are not experienced in pure form, but in
combinations. If you play the clarinet in the school band, you might want to
improve your technique simply because you enjoy playing as well as
possible—essentially a mastery orientation. But you might
also want to look
talented in the eyes of classmates—a performance orientation. Another part of
what you may wish, at least privately, is to avoid looking like a complete
failure at playing the clarinet. One of these motives may predominate over the
others, but they all may be present.
Mastery goals tend to
be associated with enjoyment of learning the material at hand, and in this
sense represent an outcome that teachers often seek for students. By definition
therefore they are a form of intrinsic motivation. As such mastery goals
have been found to be better than performance goals at sustaining students’
interest in a subject. In one review of research about learning goals, for
example, students with primarily mastery orientations toward a course they were
taking not only tended to express greater interest in the course, but also
continued to express interest well beyond the official end of the course, and
to enroll in further courses in the same subject
(Harackiewicz, et
al., 2002; Wolters, 2004).
Performance goals, on
the other hand, imply extrinsic motivation, and tend to show the mixed
effects of this orientation. A positive effect is that students with a
performance orientation do tend to get higher grades than those who express
primarily a mastery orientation. The advantage in grades occurs both in the
short term (with individual assignments) and in the long term (with overall
grade point average when graduating). But there is evidence that performance
oriented students do not actually learn material as deeply or permanently as
students who are more mastery oriented (Midgley, Kaplan, & Middleton,
2001). A possible reason is that measures of performance—such as test
scores—often reward relatively shallow memorization of information and
therefore guide performance-oriented students away from processing the
information thoughtfully or deeply. Another possible reason is that a performance
orientation, by focusing on gaining recognition as the best among peers,
encourages competition among peers. Giving and receiving help from classmates
is thus not in the self-interest of a performance-oriented student, and the
resulting isolation limits the student’s learning.
Goals that affect
achievement indirectly
Failure-avoidant
goals
As we mentioned,
failure-avoidant goals by nature undermine academic achievement. Often they are
a negative by product of the competitiveness of performance goals (Urdan,
2004). If a teacher (and sometimes also fellow students) put too much emphasis
on being the best in the class, and if interest in learning the material as
such therefore suffers, then some students may decide that success is beyond
their reach or may not be desirable in any case. The alternative—simply
avoiding failure—may seem wiser as well as more feasible. Once a student adopts
this attitude, he or she may underachieve more or less deliberately, doing only
the minimum work necessary to avoid looking foolish or to avoid serious
conflict with the teacher. Avoiding failure in this way is an example of self-handicapping—
deliberate actions and choices that the reduce chances of success. Students may
self-handicap in a number of ways; in addition to not working hard, they may
procrastinate about completing assignments, for example, or set goals that are
unrealistically high.
References
Guthrie, J.,
Wigfield, A., & Humenick, N. (2006). Influences of stimulating tasks on
reading motivation and
comprehension. Journal
of Educational Research, 99, 232-245.
Harp, S. & Mayer,
R. (1998). How seductive details do their damage. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 90,
414-434.
Harzckiewicz, J.,
Barron, K., Tauer, J., & Elliot, A. (2002). Short-term and long-term
consequences of
achievement goals. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 92, 316-320.
Hidi, S. &
Renninger, A. (2006). A four-phase model of interest development. Educational
Psychology, 41,
111-127.
Johnson, D. &
Johnson, R. (1999). Learning together and alone: Cooperative, competitive,
and
individualistic
learning, 5th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Kohn, A. (1996). No
contest: The case against competition. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Koestner, R. &
Losier, G. (2004). Distinguishing three ways of being highly motivated: a
closer look at
introjection,
identification, and intrinsic motivation. In E. Deci & R. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook
of selfdetermination
research
(pp. 101-122). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Lent, R., Brown, S.,
Nota, L., & Soresi, S. (2003). Teaching social cognitive interest and
choice hypotheses
across Holland types
in Italian high school students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, 101-118.
Lindley, L. (2006).
The paradox of self-efficacy: Research with diverse populations. Journal of
Career
Assessment,
14(1), 143-160.
Mau, W.-C. (2003).
Factors that influence persistence in science and engineering career
aspirations. Career
Development
Quarterly, 51, 234-243.
Midgley, C., Kaplan,
A., & Middleton, M. (2001). Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for
whom, and
under what
conditions, and at what cost? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77-86.
Pajares, F. &
Schunk, D. (2001). Self-beliefs and school success: Self-efficacy,
self-concept, and school
achievement. In .
Riding & S. Rayner (Eds.), Perception (pp. 239-266). London: Ablex
Publishing.
Pajares, F. &
Schunk, D. (2002). Self-beliefs in psychology and education: An historical
perspective. In J.
Aronson (Ed.), Improving
academic achievement (pp. 3-21). New York: Academic Press.
Reynolds, P. &
Symons, S. (2001). Motivational variables and children’s text search. Journal
of Educational
Psychology,
93, 14-22.
Ryan, R. & Lynch,
M. (2003). Philosophies of motivation and classroom management. In R. Curren
(Ed.),
Blackwell
companion to philosophy: A companion to the philosophy of education (pp.
260-271). New
York, NY: Blackwell.
Sanchez, C. &
Wiley, J. (2006). An examination of the seductive details effect in terms of
working memory ,capacity. Memory and Cognition, 34, 344-355.
Social Plugin